BJMB! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! Research Article!
Brazilian(Journal(of(Motor(Behavior!
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v14i3.190
others
15,19,20
participants request a relatively high frequency of feedback (ranging from an
average of 31.3% to 97% of practice trials).
The frequency of feedback chosen by participants appears to influence skill
acquisition. For example, Chiviacowsky, de Medeiros, Kaefer, Wally, and Wulf
21
compared
children who requested relatively high frequencies of feedback (i.e., 39.3%) to those who
requested relatively low frequencies of feedback (i.e., 8.4%) while learning a beanbag
tossing task. The results indicated that participants who requested relatively more
feedback performed better in the retention test compared to participants who requested
relatively less feedback. The authors argued that the benefits of self-controlled feedback
are likely mediated by the feedback frequency requested by the learners. Further, the
authors suggest that identifying instructions that impact the frequency of feedback
requests could increase the observed benefits of self-controlled feedback.
Examining the effects of instructions on skill acquisition can also provide additional
insight into the mechanisms underlying the benefits of self-controlled learning feedback.
For example, Hooyman, Wulf, and Lewthwaite
22
examined if instructions that increased
learners’ feelings of autonomy would enhance the acquisition of cricket pitching to a target
compared to controlling or neutral instructions. As mentioned earlier, increased feelings of
autonomy as a result of self-controlled feedback have been associated with enhanced
learning.
23
Three groups were exposed to either autonomy supportive, controlling, or
neutral instructions before practice begin. The results indicated that participants who
received autonomy-supportive instructions demonstrated enhanced skill acquisition
compared to participants who received controlling or neutral instructions. The autonomy-
supportive instructions also led to greater feelings of autonomy, higher self-efficacy and
positive affect. The authors argued that the instructions increased learners’ feelings of
autonomy and in turn enhanced learning. That increase in feelings of autonomy did not,
however, lead to statistically significant changes in self-controlled behavior (i.e. pacing)
during acquisition. That is, participants did not take advantage of the autonomy afforded to
them. This might have happened because participants had limited opportunity to exert
control over the learning environment (i.e., participants were only allowed to control
pacing), differences in the content of the instructions themselves (i.e., participants in the
autonomy supportive group were told “feel free to go at a pace you are comfortable with”
while participants in the controlling group were told that “you must maintain a consistent
pace”), or limited exposure to the instructions (i.e., participants were only given the
instructions once at the beginning of practice).
In the present study, we investigated if autonomy-supportive language leads to
different pattern and frequency of feedback requests and skill acquisition. Primarily, we
expected that participants exposed to language that promoted autonomy would request
more feedback than those who were exposed to more controlling instructions and that the
differences between groups in feedback requests would increase during the acquisition
phase. Secondarily, we expected that language promoting autonomy would lead to
increased retention of a motor skill compared to instructions containing more controlling
language.
METHODS
Participants and experimental groups