their views, ideas, experiments – in any of the many levels of analysis that motor learning
touches – with the following intentions. First, it is always good to remember us that the
issue of practice is still an open question – one of the most basic in the area of motor
learning. Having a special issue highlights that. Second, the special issue will shed light on
the diversity in opinions and approaches to the problem of practice. In fact, the diversity
highlights the difficulty in answering the question. Third, and final, it might be that only
through this diversity that a potential solution can be found.
FROM BASIC CONCEPTS TO CANDIDATE VIEWS
To my surprise, the expected diversity was surpassed by far. While I thought that
two to three views would be introduced through different papers, the authors (and their
different views) showed that the question of practice might not even be the question to be
answered at the moment. Ranganathan, Lee and Krishnan
5
point out that motor learning
as a phenomenon is key to many academic disciplines which, despite increased effort in
understanding the phenomenon, leads to variety of approaches to the same problem.
Such diversity might threaten common understanding as different methods, concepts and
definitions lead to a babelic situation.
To remediate the situation, Ranganathan, Lee and Krishnan
5
provide a set of
guidelines that would help researchers to solve the problem and raise awareness to the
incompatibility of “approaches” to motor learning. These guidelines range from
methodological decisions (e.g., task selection, measurement decisions) to open science
directions (e.g., pre-registration, code availability). It is of primary importance that the first
step (or “stage 0”) is “Defining what motor learning is”, in the sense that not even this has
been solved. Would be intuitive to say, therefore, that the question “what is the role of
practice in motor learning?” should await further agreement in the motor learning area first.
A second approach to my invitation was to postulate new views on the process of
motor learning – which might solve the aforementioned issues and provide an answer to
the role of practice. A first view was the hierarchical system’s view to motor learning,
discussed by Corrêa et al.
6
. Under this view, motor learning relates to formation of an
invariant macrostructure – directed to consistency and order – which constrains a variant
and flexible microstructure. According to the authors, the consideration of these two levels
allows for a hybrid view with representation and emergence (see
7
for a thorough treatment
of the issue). The authors list several insights that result from applying such approach on
the question of practice schedules.
In a similar vein, Profeta and Ugrinowitsch
8
compare the Adaptive Process
approach
9
and Specificity of Practice hypothesis
10
on the question of extensive practice.
Note that while extending practice would be a source of increased adaptability for the
former, the latter predicts a decreased capability to deal with new contexts (changed
informational resources) as there is an increase in specificity. Following the idea of
hierarchically organized open systems, they provide that studies under the Adaptive
Process approach have challenged the original results explained by the Specificity of
Practice. Under this, they consider how methodological aspects differ and how the same
aspect in practice (extensive practice) can result in different outcomes when using different
perspectives.
Another view, now from an ecological standpoint (ecological dynamics
11
),