BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
1 of 14
Brief History of the Uncontrolled Manifold Hypothesis and Its Role in Motor Control
MARK L. LATASH1
1 Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
Correspondence to: Mark L. Latash
Department of Kinesiology, Rec.Hall-267, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA
tel: (814) 863-5374
email: mll11@psu.edu
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
HIGHLIGHTS
The concept of uncontrolled manifold is readily
compatible with the principle of abundance
The UCM concept can be applied to different effectors
from a muscle to the whole body
The UCM concept offers insightful applications to
movement disorders
This concept requires generalization to spaces of
neural control variables
ABBREVIATIONS
ASAs Anticipatory synergy adjustments
ASASS Steady-State ASA
ASATR Transient ASA
C- Coactivation command
E1 Elemental variable 1
E2 Elemental variable 2
Fx Effector producing force
J Jacobian matrix
k Apparent stiffness
ME Motor equivalent
ORT Orthogonal
PD Parkinson’s disease
R- Reciprocal command
RC Reference Coordinates
UCM Uncontrolled manifold
VF Virtual finger
VUCM Inter-trial variance components along the
UCM
VORT Inter-trial variance components along the
ORT
X Spatial coordinate
∆V Synergy index
PUBLICATION DATA
Received 18 07 2024
Accepted 09 09 2024
Published 31 10 2024
BACKGROUND: The apparent problem of motor redundancy was replaced by the principle of
abundance and turned into a theoretical framework and associated toolbox for exploration of
performance-stabilizing synergies.
AIM and METHOD: We review briefly the development of the main methods within the UCM
framework and some of the main findings, both basic and clinical. The UCM framework is
naturally merged with the theory of hierarchical movement control with spatial referent
coordinates.
RESULTS: The UCM framework has established itself as a productive framework for the
analysis of movement control, in particular as related to stability of salient performance
variables. It led to the discovery of novel phenomena such as trade-offs within hierarchical
systems, anticipatory synergy adjustments, synergies within systems of different complexity
from single muscles to the whole body. It has also led to promising results offering sensitive
biomarkers to various neurological disorders. Recent experiments suggest the existence of
three main levels of organization of performance-stabilizing synergies tentatively associated
with cortical, subcortical, and spinal circuitry.
CONCLUSION: Currently, this approach is in its adulthood. Further progress may be
expected in focusing on spaces of neural control variables, developing the method for analysis
across species, and expanding the range and depth of clinical studies.
KEYWORDS: Abundance | Stability | Synergy | Referent coordinate | Hierarchy
To the memory of John P. Scholz A friend, a gentleman, and a scientist
LIFE BEFORE UCM
It is hard to believe that a quarter of a century has passed since the seminal publication by John Scholz and Gregor Schöner in
1999 1 inaugurating the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis. The origins of this hypothesis can be traced back to the classical study
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
2 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
of Nikolai Bernstein 2 of professional blacksmiths who performed their labor movement hitting the chisel with the hammer multiple
times. Bernstein quantified the trajectories of the tip of the hammer and of individual arm joints and claimed that the former showed the
smallest inter-trial variability. Since, clearly, the brain could only send signals to muscles crossing the joints, not to the hammer, the result
implied that the joints compensated for each other errors (deviations from the mean trajectories) across trials to keep the hammer
trajectory relatively consistent. In 1994, we discussed with Gregor Schöner how Bernstein could compare variability of the tip of the
hammer measured in spatial Euclidian coordinates with variability in the joint space measured in angular units. Clearly, some kind of
mapping between these two spaces was necessary to reach the main Bernstein conclusion. The necessity of such mapping was explicitly
formulated by Gregor Schöner in a paper published in 1995 3.
The general idea of error compensation among effectors had been expressed in several studies preceding the introduction of
the UCM concept. Such studies involved either external perturbations applied to one or a few of the effectors or self-generated actions
perturbing the salient task-specific variable, or simply repetitive movements without any explicit perturbations. They explored a range of
actions including speech 4, precision grip 5, multi-joint pointing 6, and multi-finger accurate force production 7. All the mentioned studies
have shown that task-specific performance variables could show relatively small deviations in the presence of relatively large changes in
the contributions of elements (digits, joints, articulators, etc.).
For many years, general theoretical conceptualization of the phenomena of error compensation has been elusive. One of the
problems can be traced back to another important contribution by Bernstein, his formulation of the problem of motor redundancy. In any
action, multiple elements are involved at different levels of analysis (joints, digits, muscles, articulators, etc.), producing a large number of
elemental variables, more than the number of task constraints. In any given realization, a specific combination of the elemental variables
is observed. How does the brain select those specific combinations from the infinite number of solutions? Bernstein viewed this as a
central problem of motor control and saw the solution in the elimination of redundant degrees-of-freedom 8 (see also 9). This view
dominated the field for decades leading to the development and application of the concept of optimization to voluntary movements
(reviewed in 10-12).
At closer examination, the problem of motor redundancy seems to be ill-formulated (reviewed in 13-15). It is not inherent to the
neural control of movement but reflects specific levels of analysis selected by individual researchers. For example, the problem of
pressing with a finger and producing a certain force level is non-redundant at the finger level (in contrast, for example, to the same task
performed while pressing with four fingers). The same problem is redundant at the level of muscle involvement. Another example: Muscle
co-contraction has been viewed as a means of alleviating the problem of motor redundancy at the joint configuration level by “freezing”
joints (cf. 16), while it obviously leads to making this problem worse at the level of muscle activation. Assuming that this problem is
inherent to brain processes involved in the neural control of movement requires committing to a motor control theory. We will return to
this issue based on the theory of control with spatial referent coordinates (reviewed in 17), a development of the equilibrium-point
hypothesis 18,19.
An alternative view on the apparent excess of elemental variables was suggested as the principle of abundance 13,20. According
to this principle, the numerous elemental variables (at all levels) are not sources of computational problems for the brain but important
parts of mechanisms that allow to provide desired degree of dynamical stability for salient task-specific performance variables. This idea
was implied in the formulation of the UCM hypothesis: No degrees-of-freedom are ever eliminated, they are all used to provide dynamical
stability of performance. Note that both features, addressing the problem of motor redundancy and ensuring dynamical stability of
performance, have been discussed by Bernstein as originating at the same level of movement construction, the Level of Synergies. This
term comes from a Greek word combination meaning “work together”, and it has been used for ages in numerous fields from theology to
motor control. Further, we will use the term synergy to address both aspects, grouping of elements at the selected level of analysis and
covariation of elemental variables contributing to stability of performance (reviewed in 8,15).
THE UCM-BASED TOOLBOX
Formal analysis of synergies, within the UCM framework, starts with defining mapping between the spaces of elemental
variables (those produced by elements at the selected level of analysis) and a potentially important performance variable, which can be
multi-dimensional. In a linear approximation, this has been done using the matrix of partial derivatives of the selected performance
variable with respect to the elemental variables, i.e., the Jacobian matrix (J). It is assumed that the average across repetitive trials value
(time series) of the performance variable represents its desired value (time series), and that deviations from this value are reflections of
its imperfect stability under the action of unpredictable factors, both extrinsic and intrinsic. It is also assumed that the average point (time
series) in the multi-dimensional space of elemental variables represents a preferred solution for the problem of producing the
performance variable. Further, for any given value of the performance variable, a solution subspace (the UCM) is approximated as the
null-space of the J, i.e., a subspace where small deviations of elemental variables have no effect on the performance variable.
Figure 1 illustrates a simple task of producing a desired value of the sum of two elemental variables (E1 and E2). The J matrix
for this task is [1 1], and its null-space is shown as the slanted line corresponding to the equation E1 + E2 = C. An average across multiple
trials solution is shown as the black point. The thin lines show potential fields along the UCM and along the orthogonal to the UCM
subspace (ORT) illustrating that the ORT direction shows higher stability as compared to the UCM.
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
3 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
Figure 1. A schematic illustration of a task of producing a constant value of the sum of two elemental variables (E1 + E2 = C). The solutions space is shown as the solid
slanted line. An average across multiple trials solutions is shown as the black point. The curved lines show potential fields along the UCM and along the orthogonal to the
UCM subspace (ORT), UCM and ORT. Note that variance along the UCM is larger than along the ORT (VUCM > VORT).
The most commonly used method of analysis has compared inter-trial variance components along the UCM and along ORT,
VUCM and VORT, normalized per dimension in each subspace. It has been assumed that, if the selected performance variable is stabilized
by some neural mechanisms, VUCM is expected to be larger than VORT, i.e., the unavoidable inter-trial variance is channeled into the space
where deviations of elemental variables have no effect on the performance variable. Commonly, the two metrics, VUCM and VORT, have
been combined into a single metric, a synergy index (∆V) reflecting the relative amounts of the two variance components in the total
amount of variance in the space of elemental variables. VUCM has been addressed informally as “good variance” because large amounts
of VUCM both reflect stability of the salient performance variable and allow performing secondary tasks with the same set of elemental
variables without detrimental effects on that salient performance variable.
Another method quantified and compared deviations within UCM and ORT following a brief action or a quick response to an
external perturbation. Deviations within the UCM are motor equivalent (ME) in a sense that they, by definition, lead to no change in the
salient performance variable. In other words, these deviations are wasteful if the task is to change the performance variable quickly and
efficiently, for example with minimal expenditure of metabolic energy a common currency within the body (cf. 21) and a commonly used
cost function within optimization approaches 11,22,23. A number of studies have shown that quick corrections of actions in response to
unexpected perturbations have very large ME components as compared to the non-ME components, which are required to produce
corrections 24-26. These apparently wasteful components of actions have been interpreted as consequences of much lower stability along
the UCM.
The two pairs of outcome variables, {VUCM; VORT} and {ME; non-ME}, are expected to correlate if the data are sampled from a
single distribution (cf. 27). Such correlations have indeed been shown in experiments 28, although not without exceptions 29, The
exceptions have been interpreted as consequences of intentional corrections by the subjects resulting in sampling from different
distributions. The relative usefulness of the two pairs of metrics remains to be explored in detail. In particular, the number of trials to
reach a criterion of statistical robustness is smaller for {ME; non-ME} as compared to {VUCM; VORT} 30 making quantification of motor
equivalence attractive in clinical studies when participants may not be able to perform multiple trials per conditions to allow estimation of
the variance indices.
APPLICATIONS TO SPACES OF MECHANICAL AND ELECTROPHYSIOLOGICAL VARIABLES
Early studies applied the analysis of variance within the UCM framework to spaces of kinematic 1,31 and kinetic 32,33 elemental
variables. These applications were relatively straightforward since the J matrix could be computed from the configuration of the effector.
There were, however, hidden problems. Drawing conclusion relevant to the neural control of movement based on results of such studies
assumes that, in the absence of a special control process, the data distributions are expected to be spherical. This is far from being
obvious. For example, the presence of biarticular and multi-articular muscles and inter-joint reflexes makes joint rotations coupled. So,
analysis in a joint configuration space may be expected to produce non-spherical data distributions in the absence of any specific
synergies. By chance, such distributions can be elongated along the UCM computed for a performance variable or orthogonal to it
leading to spurious conclusions. How practically important are these factors? In particular, can humans move one joint at a time? There
are no data answering these questions. The situation is even more complicated for analysis in spaces of segmental angles because,
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
4 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
obviously, moving a single proximal joint alone leads to changes in the segmental angles of all the more distal segments. These built-in
covariations may also lead to spurious conclusions. It is better to avoid analysis in spaces where unavoidable covariation of elemental
variables is produced by peripheral, including biomechanical, factors.
The situation becomes even more complicated in the analysis of multi-digit actions because of the phenomenon of enslaving 34,
which makes digit forces non-independent. Originally, to account for finger enslaving, finger forces were transformed into modes,
hypothetical central variables that could be manipulated by the brain one at a time 32,35. Such analyses implied that enslaving was a
robust phenomenon and, after being defined on a set of tasks, it could be applied to other tasks. Recent studies have shown, however,
that enslaving tends to drift (increase) with time during constant finger force production tasks 36,37. Whether using finger modes should be
preferred as compared to analysis in finger force spaces remains an open issue.
Application of the UCM framework to spaces of electrophysiological variables, such as EMG indices, led to the emergence of
an auxiliary toolbox. It would be naïve to assume that the brain manipulates a set of neural variables directed at individual muscles. So,
the first step in those studies has been defining muscle groups that show parallel changes in the activation levels of individual muscles.
This has been done with the help of various matrix factorization methods, in particular the principal component analysis, which leads to a
set of orthogonal eigenvectors an advantage for further normalization of the variance indices. Such groups have been addressed with
various terms such as factors, primitives, modes, and synergies. Further, since the mapping between indices of EMG (such as mV or
mVs) and mechanical variables directly relevant to the task is typically unknown, the only practical approach has been to discover the J
matrix using experimental data, commonly with the help of multiple linear regression techniques 38,39. Relatively recently, the UCM-based
analysis has been applied to studies of performance-stabilizing synergies in spaces of firing frequencies of individual motor units within a
muscle and across agonist-antagonist muscle pairs 40 (reviewed in 41). Both features of synergies were demonstrated: Grouping motor
units into a small number of groups (MU-modes) and stabilization of the task-specific performance variable organized in the spaces of
MU-modes, i.e., VUCM > VORT.
APPLICATIONS TO SPACES OF HYPOTHETICAL CONTROL VARIABLES
All the aforementioned studies analyzed inter-trial variance and/or motor equivalence in spaces of peripheral elemental
variables. This has always been a potential weakness of the approach because peripheral mechanical and muscle activation variables
cannot in principle be prescribed by the brain as emphasized by many researchers starting with Bernstein 8. In particular, the discovered
patterns could be consequences of correlated changes in the reflex contributions to muscle activation levels. To extend the method to
spaces of control variables, one had to commit to a theory of motor control, which specifies such variables explicitly. This has been done
within the framework of the theory of control with spatial referent coordinates (RCs, linear or angular; reviewed in 17,42). Within this
framework, the control of any effector can be described with two basic commands, the reciprocal command (R-command) and
coactivation command (C-command). The R-command defines spatial coordinate where the net force by the agonist and antagonist
muscles is zero, i.e., its RC. The C-command defines the range where both agonist and antagonist muscles are active simultaneously.
This is illustrated in Figure 2 for an effector producing force FX along a spatial coordinate X. Note that, at the level of mechanics,
changing the C-command leads to changes in the shape of the resultant FX(X) characteristic or, in a linear approximation, the apparent
stiffness (k) of the effector (cf. 43).
Figure 2. An effector produces force FX along a spatial coordinate X. The thin curves show force-coordinate characteristics for two opposing muscles, agonist (positive
force values) and antagonist (negative force values). They were addressed as “invariant characteristics” in the original publications 18,19. The agonist and antagonist muscle
groups are controlled by setting referent coordinates, RCAG and RCANT. At the level of mechanics, changing the R-command leads to shifts in the RC for the effector.
Changes in the C-command leads, in a linear approximation, to changes in the apparent stiffness (k) of the effector.
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
5 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
The presence of the two basic commands makes the control of any effector abundant with a possibility of performance-
stabilizing synergies. However, the two commands are not directly measurable, and so far, studies of synergies at the level of control
have been limited to using mechanical proxies of the two commands, the intercept and slope of the FX(X) characteristic. Such studies
have been informative showing performance-stabilizing synergies for different effectors, from single fingers to the whole body 44,45. The
UCM in the {RC; k} space stabilizing force by an effector is typically hyperbolic and does not allow linearization. This has been another
problem preventing researchers from using analysis of inter-trial variance. Instead, hyperbolic regression and randomization methods (cf.
46) have been used to quantify synergies in the {RC; k } space.
The concept of control with spatial RCs can be applied to different effectors, from the whole body to individual muscles and
motor units (reviewed in 14,42). Figure 3 illustrates a hypothetical hierarchy involved in the control of a multi-joint effector. Note that the
dimensionality of control increases from the task level down to the levels of specific effectors, joints, digits, muscles, and motor units.
These few-to-many transformations afford the possibility of organizing synergies stabilizing higher-level control variables by co-varied
involvement of lower-level variables. This can potentially be based on feedback loops from peripheral sensory endings as well as those
within the central nervous system shown by arrows. The scheme in Figure 3 has to be taken with a big grain of salt. It is motivated by the
anatomy of the human body, and some of the levels may be apparent, not real. For example, a study of spino-cerebellar pathways in cats
has shown modulation of those signals not with joint angles, which have dedicated sensors, but with higher-order variables such as “leg
length” and “leg orientation”, which do not have dedicated sensors 47.
Figure 3. A schematic illustration of the control in a hierarchy involved in the control of an effector. Note that the dimensionality o f control (referent coordinates, RC)
increases from the task level down to the levels of specific joints, digits, muscles, and motor units. Back-coupling loops stabilizing behavior from peripheral sensory endings
and within the central nervous system are shown with arrows.
The relations between the theory of control with RCs and the UCM-based concept of synergy are non-trivial. On the one hand,
within the hierarchical scheme in Figure 3, synergies stabilizing performance variables desired by the actor and indirectly encoded in the
low-dimensional task-level RC time functions can be organized using feedback loops both within the central nervous system, from
abundant sets of lower-level RC time functions, and from peripheral sensory endings. On the other hand, what matters for the actor is
stability of salient performance variable, not necessarily its source. So, it is possible that peripheral factors, including anatomical and
biomechanical ones, may drive covariations of elemental variables stabilizing performance with minimal contribution from the neural
control levels. In most cited studies, it has been assumed that action stability is primarily a function of the neural control, i.e., of the RC
hierarchy, but the role of peripheral factors remains relatively underexplored.
The organization of synergic control in a hierarchical system is non-trivial. Indeed, consider only two levels of a hierarchy
(Figure 4). To have a synergy stabilizing performance at the upper level, VUCM has to be relatively large, which requires relatively large
inter-trial variance of the elements. At the lower level, variance of each of the elements is, by definition, VORT. This large VORT requires
very large VUCM to ensure synergic control. This may be problematic, although not impossible. A study of two-hand, multi-finger pressing
tasks showed that, indeed, in such tasks no synergies were seen stabilizing the contribution of each individual hand, while there were
strong synergies at the two-hand level 48. During prehensile tasks performed using the prismatic grip the thumb opposing the four
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
6 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
fingers the control is commonly viewed as hierarchical. At the top level, the task is shared between the thumb and virtual finger” (VF,
an imagined digit with the force/moment vector equal to the summed vectors of the individual fingers, 49). At the lower level, the VF action
is shared among the four actual fingers. A study of synergies stabilizing force and moment components has shown that some of them
were stabilized at one level only, while others at both levels 50. Such studies may be useful as windows into the salient control levels,
which may differ from the apparent anatomy-based levels shown in Figure 4.
Figure 4. Consider the task of keeping constant the sum of two effectors (E1 and E2), when the output of each effector is the sum of two lower-level effectors, e11 + e12
and e21 + e22. A synergy stabilizing performance implies large VUCM, which requires large inter-trial variance of the elements, VE1 and VE2. At the lower level, variance of
each of the elements is, by definition, VORT. This creates a problem for arranging synergies at both levels.
ANTICIPATORY CHANGES IN ACTION STABILITY
The UCM-based method of analysis of stability has led to several discoveries. Some of them have already been mentioned.
Here we address a group of phenomena related to anticipation in motor behavior. High stability of salient variables may be functionally
important during steady-state or slow actions. If one wants to change a performance variable quickly, strong synergies stabilizing this
variable become counter-productive. This has been discussed in detail as a stability-agility trade-off (reviewed in 51). Evolutionary success
required controlled stability of action, and a number of studies have shown that humans can adjust action stability independently of other
action characteristics, as seen, for example, in average across trials performance. These phenomena have been addressed as
anticipatory synergy adjustments (ASAs, reviewed in 14,15).
There are two types of ASAs. The first is associated with a drop in the index of performance-stabilizing synergy seen in young
healthy persons 300±100 ms prior to a self-initiated quick action or reaction to a predictable perturbation 52,53. The second type
represents a drop in the synergy index (the normalized difference between VUCM and VORT) during steady-state performance under
conditions that a quick action or a change in the ongoing action may be required at an unpredictable time in future or in only some of the
trials 54-56. These have been labeled as transient ASAs (ASATR) and steady-state ASAs (ASASS), respectively. They are illustrated in
Figure 5 using the earlier example of two elements contributing to a common task. Panel A shows the task of producing a constant output
with a possibility of unexpected changes in the output magnitude followed by a self-initiation quick change in (E1 + E2). Panel B shows
cartoon inter-trial distributions of the data points for a steady-state action in the absence of any changes in performance, during steady-
state when an unexpected target could emerge, and prior to the self-initiated quick change in (E1 + E2). Panel C illustrates the same data
assuming that stability of action reflects depth of a potential field.
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
7 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
Figure 5. A: The task of producing a constant output of two elements (E1 + E2 = C) with a possibility of unexpected changes in the output magnitude followed by a self-
initiation quick change in (E1 + E2). B: Inter-trial distributions of the data points for a steady-state action in the absence of any changes in performance (the dark ellipse),
when an unexpected target could emerge (the lighter ellipse), and prior to the self-initiated quick action (the very light ellipse). C: An illustration of the same data assuming
that stability of action reflects depth of a potential field, .
ASASS can be seen with the naked eye in some athletic competitions. For example, a tennis player getting ready for a powerful
serve and a goalkeeper preparing for a penalty shot both show visibly increased postural sway reflecting a decrease in postural stability.
This facilitates the initiation of a fast action independently of its direction. Relative invariance of ASATR to action direction has also been
shown experimentally 57.
Theoretically, a drop in the synergy index may be due to an increase in VORT and/or a drop in VUCM; both scenarios have been
documented in the mentioned papers. Recently, sensitivity of both ASATR and ASASS to speed of a future action has been documented 58:
Both types of ASAs become smaller and may even disappear when the planned upcoming action is slower as compared to very fast
actions used in most of the earlier studies. This is potentially an important finding for clinical applications when control participants
typically are able to perform faster actions as compared to groups of patients with impaired neural control of movements. We will return to
clinical applications of ASA indices a bit later.
WHERE DOES THE UCM COME FROM?
While the UCM concept and associated methods of analysis involve computational steps, these computations have not been
assumed to happen within the central nervous system, only in the minds of the researchers. Just like equations in classical physics
involve computations, these are never assumed to reside in the objects to which they apply. From the very beginning, the controlled
stability of actions has been assumed to reflect physical (including physiological) processes within the body. The most developed
accounts for the UCM concept have been developed by the group of Gregor Schöner 59,60, which also incorporate the ideas of control with
spatial RCs. While the mapping between the assumed processes and neurophysiological structures remains speculative, the basic ideas
of back-coupling and the role of feedback circuits within the central nervous system and from peripheral receptors are important for
conceptualization of the UCM.
In particular, an important role of short-latency feedback circuits in ensuring stability of actions has been suggested nearly
twenty years ago 61. Recent studies of intra-muscle synergies corroborate this idea and suggest that intra-spinal and reflex-based circuits
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
8 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
contribute to movement stability across tasks and effectors (reviewed in 51) in addition to supraspinal synergic mechanisms, which are
more task-specific and sensitive to conditions of movement execution, in particular to the presence of visual feedback on salient
variables. Recent studies of intra-muscle and multi-effector (multi-muscle) synergies have strongly suggested the existence of at least
two classes of performance-stabilizing synergies. Intra-muscle synergies stabilize reflex-induced changes in performance, show no
effects of hemispheric dominance, and are robust independently of the availability of visual feedback 62-64. In contrast, multi-effector
synergies show no stabilization of reflex-based changes in performance, show strong effects of dominance (compatible with the dynamic
dominance hypothesis, 65), and are highly sensitive to visual feedback. For example, turning off the visual feedback on the force produced
by the four fingers of a hand leads to unintentional force drifts (cf. 66,67) accompanied by the disappearance of multi-finger force-stabilizing
synergies with no effects on intra-muscle synergies 64.
It seems feasible that the two classes of synergies reflect different levels of back-coupling hypothesized within the scheme by
Martin and colleagues 60, those targeting spinal levels of control and those updating the RC functions at higher levels. As far as
supraspinal mechanisms of synergies are concerned, a number of studies have shown high sensitivity of synergies to disorders of
subcortical circuitry in such conditions as Parkinson’s disease, multi-system atrophy, and multiple sclerosis (reviewed in 68). In contrast,
effects of cortical stroke have been variable ranging from no effects on synergy indices to weakening of the synergies 69-72 (reviewed in
68,73).
A recent study explored the indices of synergies at three levels during multi-finger accurate force production tasks 74. In addition
to analysis of intra-muscle and multi-finger force-stabilizing synergies, this study quantified the synergies at the {RC; k} level reflecting the
hypothetical R- and C-commands. Force-stabilizing synergies were found at all three levels of analysis. However, no correlations were
found across the synergy indices quantified at the three levels suggesting their independent origins. A hypothetical scheme reflecting
these findings is illustrated in Figure 6. It seems feasible that the {RC; k} synergies are hierarchically the highest, possibly involving pre-
M1 cortical levels, followed by the multi-finger synergies based on subcortical circuitry, and by intra-muscle synergies based on spinal
circuitry. The presence of multiple parallel mechanisms contributing to dynamical stability of movements ensures that no major disruption
happens when one of these mechanisms stops contributing to action for some reason. For example, as mentioned earlier, turning visual
feedback off during accurate force production tasks leads to loss of force stability reflected in its drifts. On the other hand, the drifts are
not erratic but relatively consistent in their patterns and limited in magnitude suggesting that stability is not completely lost but rather
attenuated.
Figure 6. Three hypothetical levels of ensuring stability of action by an effector and the hypothetical neurophysiological substrates. Force by an effector (e.g., a hand) may
be stabilized at the level of R- and C-commands, at the level of sharing it across the fingers (I index, M middle, R ring, and L little), and at the level of involvement of
the motor unit groups (MU-modes).
SUBCLINICAL AND CLINICAL APPLICATIONS
Many conditions, from natural aging to neurological disorders, are associated with problems related to movement stability.
Examples range from mishandling hand-held objects to ataxic limb movements, and to problems with postural stability. Less obviously,
problems with movement initiation may also be related to impaired control of stability, the most obvious example may be episodes of
freezing in Parkinson’s disease. In this case, however, they are related to impaired ability to attenuate stability of salient performance
variables and make it difficult to initiate properly timed quick changes in the performance variables (see the earlier example of ASAs).
The two aspects of the impaired control of movement stability have been addressed as impaired stability and impaired agility (reviewed in
68).
Indices of stability quantified within the UCM framework show high sensitivity to relatively mild changes in the control of
movements, e.g., those seen under atypical development, under fatigue, with healthy aging, and in populations at high risk for
neurological disorders (reviewed in 15). In particular, persons with Down syndrome show reduced indices of multi-finger synergies during
accurate force production 75, muscle fatigue can lead to increased multi-effector synergy indices and reduced intra-muscle synergies 76,77,
older persons show reduced synergy indices during steady-state tasks and reduced ASAs in preparation to quick actions 78,79, and
professional welders who are predisposed to basal ganglia disorders due to the accumulation of manganese and iron in the brain show
smaller synergy indices 80.
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
9 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
The high sensitivity of synergy indices to Parkinson’s disease (PD) has been shown in studies, which demonstrated
significantly changed indices of both performance-stabilizing synergies and ASAs in effectors and tasks apparently unaffected by the
disease (according to the clinical examination) 81,82 and in de novo PD patients 83,84. These indices are also differentially sensitive to both
dopamine-replacement treatment and deep brain stimulation in PD 85-87. Overall, even though clinical studies using the UCM-based
methods are still relatively few, they promise important practical impact as behavioral biomarkers at early stages of neurological disorders
and as predictors of both treatment and disease progression, e.g., predicting episodes of freezing in PD 73,88.
It remains mostly unknown if training can lead to improved control of the action stability. So far, various studies have shown that
at early stages of practicing an unusual task, synergies stabilizing performance variables strengthen, primarily due to the drop in VORT,
and at later stages they can weaken, primarily due to a disproportionate drop in VUCM 89-91. In conditions challenging stability, an increase
in VUCM can accompany a drop in VORT 92,93 (cf. 94). This is encouraging given that a number of clinical studies have documented reduced
amounts of VUCM leading to low synergy indices 82,95. So, designing studies to encourage higher VUCM seems a promising strategy.
Unfortunately, so far, transfer of the effects of practice to more functional tasks has been limited (reviewed in 96).
THE FUTURE OF THE UCM
One of the most difficult aspects of understanding and exploring the UCM concept is separating underlying hypotheses from
the associated toolbox. The primary hypothesis is that the central nervous system uses abundant sets of elemental variables to ensure
task-specific dynamical stability of salient performance variables. Secondary hypotheses have emerged based on experimental
explorations of the primary hypothesis. They relate, in particular, to ability of the brain to modulate stability of action depending on plans
and expectations (as reflected in ASAs), changes in the ability to ensure proper stability properties of movements with subclinical (fatigue
and aging) and clinical states, the role of timing and spatial errors in the dynamical stability 97,98, and a few others. So far, the toolbox to
explore these hypotheses has most frequently included the analysis of the structure of inter-trial variance and of motor equivalence.
Clearly, it is unproductive to try to disprove the toolbox, although delineating its limits of application and reliability of the outcome
variables is important. In contrast, trying to disprove specific hypotheses is potentially very valuable and can lead to their refinement or
even falsification and replacement by a different theoretical framework.
As of now, the process account of the UCM-associated stability of movements 60 provides the most useful framework and
guidelines for future analysis. The most obvious gap in our knowledge is related to only speculative links of the assumed processes to
neurophysiological substrate. The current guesses are based primarily on a handful of studies using the UCM-based analysis in patient
populations. Very few studies have applied this method to studies of movement stability in animals (e.g., 99,100). Potentially, animal studies
can provide more direct information on the underlying neurophysiological circuitry given the possibility of performing invasive procedures
and more direct recordings.
One of the challenges in analysis of synergies is shifting from sets of elemental variables at the level of performance (which are
typically selected subjectively, commonly based on the available equipment) to sets of theory-based control variables such as RCs. The
first described attempts have provided promising results but the available tools for recording control variables (s, RCs, R-command, and
C-command) and their changes in real time are currently limited and based on a number of assumptions and simplifications. So far,
mechanical and EMG-based proxies of those variables have been used. Maybe, combining analysis of both mechanical and
electrophysiological variables could lead to progress in this field.
The idea of hierarchical control with RCs (see Figure 4) raises a problem related to the described trade-offs between synergies
at different levels of a hierarchy. Recent studies have suggested the existence of three levels at which synergies are organized, the task
level of the R- and C-commands, the level of sharing the task across effectors (such as limbs and digits), and the level of stabilizing
action of individual muscles 64,74. The presence of multiple circuits stabilizing action is not surprising given the obvious importance of
action stability in the process of evolution. Other levels, e.g., those involved in stabilizing movement in individual joints, may be apparent,
not real, unless single-joint movement represents a salient task component. A related issue is the unintentional drifts in performance that
have been reported during isometric force production tasks (reviewed in 15). So far, studies of such drifts within the UCM-framework have
reported disappearance of synergies at the level of effector involvement, dramatic reduction of synergies at the level of the R- and C-
commands, and no effects on motor unit-based synergies 64,101. These studies have to be reproduced using a variety of tasks. The
differential sensitivity of circuits involved in action stability may be an important factor defining specific features of movement disorders,
but such studies have not been performed yet.
So far, most UCM-based studies have explored indices of the relative amounts of inter-trial variance or displacement within the
UCM and ORT. Less attention has been paid to analysis of data within the UCM (cf. 102), although the importance of reduced VUCM has
been emphasized in several clinical studies 86,95. It seems that excessive stability along the UCM (as reflected in low VUCM), i.e., preferring
stereotypical solutions, is highly important in its effects on stability of everyday movements. A related underexplored issue is the structure
of variance within the UCM. It is possible that some directions within the UCM are stabilized more than others reflecting importance of
other variables in addition to the variables for which the UCM was computed. Note that the UCM-based analysis is always subjective: It
starts with selecting a performance variable that is seen by the researcher as salient for the task. A number of early studies have shown,
however, that the brain can reformulate the task based on its past experience and other criteria and stabilize a variable that has not been
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
10 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
mentioned in the task formulation 32,33. Maybe combining the UCM-based toolbox with more objective analysis of the structure of
variance, e.g., the principal component analysis, can help discover hidden salient variables.
An aspect of synergic control that has not been explored in detail is the apparent trade-off among three properties of
movements, their stability (as reflected in the structure of variance, VUCM > VORT), optimality (sticking to a criterion that defines average
across trials sharing of the salient variable across elemental variables), and agility (ability to change a salient variable quickly) (reviewed
in 51). Such trade-offs have been invoked, in particular, in studies of ASAs and synergy indices in the dominant and non-dominant
extremities 103,104 and in studies of individual digits of the human hand 105. Can such inherent trade-offs be avoided? Can a person be both
optimal, agile, and stable? We do not know answers to these questions, which are obviously relevant for such diverse fields as athletics
and motor rehabilitation.
There are plenty of unsolved methodological issues related to analysis within the UCM framework. Some of them, such as
reliability of the main outcome indices, the number of trials required to reach a criterion of statistical robustness, and a few others have
been addressed recently 30,106,107. A number of issues, however, remain acknowledged but not analyzed in sufficient detail. These include
the linearization using the null-space of the J matrix in most studies, assumptions that the J matrix does not change significantly within
the selected time (or phase) window of analysis, potential interdependence among elemental variables unrelated to the task, etc.
Applied studies performed within the UCM-based framework are still few and fragmented despite the obvious functional
importance of the proper control of action stability. Very little is known about changes in the neural control of movement stability with
typical and atypical development. In contrast, this method has been applied at the other end of the spectrum, to studies of aging
demonstrating impairment in indices of both movement stability and its adjustments in preparation to quick actions (ASAs). Studies of
neurological patients have demonstrated the sensitivity of the indices quantified within the UCM framework to a variety of disorders.
However, changes in these indices with disease progression, pharmacological therapy, brain stimulation, and rehabilitation are next to
unknown with only a handful of exceptions. This field is waiting to be adequately developed.
Very little is known about changes in indices of stability with athletic training and motor rehabilitation although, obviously,
developing better stability of actions and better agility could be very beneficial in various sports and in recovery of functional everyday
movements. In laboratory conditions, practice has been shown to lead to major changes in the indices of synergies, in particular on VUCM.
In contrast, no studies have explored potential effects of practice on ASAs.
Overall, the UCM-based approach seems to be at a stage of maturity. It is considered seriously by most researchers as both a
viable theoretical framework and useful toolbox to analyze the neural control of movement stability. It is up to next generation of
researchers to address the missing pieces of the puzzle and to develop this approach and its applications. This would require deep
understanding of the main aspects of the approach and the associated fields such as linear algebra, statistics, and neurophysiology:
Challenging but not impossible.
REFERENCES
1. Scholz JP, Schöner G. The uncontrolled manifold concept: Identifying control variables for a functional task. Experimental Brain Research, 1999;
126:289-306. doi: 10.1007/s002210050738
2. Bernstein NA. A new method of mirror cyclographie and its application towards the study of labor movements during work on a workbench. Hygiene,
Safety and Pathology of Labor, 1930;# 5: 3-9, and # 6:3-11. (in Russian).
3. Schöner G. Recent developments and problems in human movement science and their conceptual implications. Ecological Psychology, 1995; 8:
291-314. doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco0704_5
4. Abbs JH, Gracco VL. Control of complex motor gestures: Orofacial muscle responses to load perturbations of the lip during speech. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 1984; 51:705-723. doi: 10.1152/jn.1984.51.4.705
5. Cole KJ, Abbs JH. Kinematic and electromyographic responses to perturbation of a rapid grasp. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1987; 57:1498-1510.
doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.57.5.1498
6. Jaric S, Latash ML. Learning a motor task involving obstacles by a multi-joint, redundant limb: Two synergies within one movement. Journal of
Electromyography and Kinesiology, 1998; 8:169-176. doi: 10.1016/s1050-6411(97)00017-5
7. Latash ML, Li Z-M, Zatsiorsky VM. A principle of error compensation studied within a task of force production by a redundant set of fingers.
Experimental Brain Research, 1998; 122: 131-138. doi: 10.1007/s002210050500
8. Bernstein NA. On the construction of movements. Medgiz: Moscow (in Russian). English translation is in: Latash ML (Ed.) (2020) Bernstein’s
Construction of Movements. 1947, Routledge: Abingdon, UK.
9. Turvey MT. Coordination. American Psychologist, 1990; 45: 938953. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.45.8.938
10. Seif-Naraghi AH, Winters JM. Optimized strategies for scaling goal-directed dynamic limb movements. In: Winters JM, Woo SL-Y (Eds.) Multiple
Muscle Systems. Biomechanics and Movement Organization, 1990; pp. 312-334, Springer-Verlag: New York.
11. Prilutsky BI, Zatsiorsky VM. Optimization-based models of muscle coordination. Exercise and Sport Science Reviews, 2002; 30: 32-38. doi:
10.1097/00003677-200201000-00007
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
11 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
12. Diedrichsen J, Shadmehr R, Ivry RB. The coordination of movement: optimal feedback control and beyond. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2010; 14:
31-39. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.11.004
13. Latash ML. The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy). Experimental Brain Research, 2012; 217: 1-5. doi: 10.1007/s00221-
012-3000-4
14. Latash ML. Physics of Biological Action and Perception. 2019; Academic Press: New York, NY.
15. Latash ML. One more time about motor (and non-motor) synergies. Experimental Brain Research, 2021; 239: 2951-2967. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-
06188-4
16. Vereijken B, van Emmerick REA, Whiting HTA, Newell KM. Free(z)ing degrees of freedom in skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 1992; 24:
133-142. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941608
17. Feldman AG. Referent control of action and perception: Challenging conventional theories in behavioral science. 2015; Springer, NY.
18. Feldman AG. Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. II. Controllable parameters of
the muscle. Biophysics, 1966; 11: 565-578.
19. Feldman AG. Once more on the equilibrium-point hypothesis ( -model) for motor control. Journal of Motor Behavior, 1986; 18: 17-54. doi:
10.1080/00222895.1986.10735369
20. Gelfand IM, Latash ML. On the problem of adequate language in movement science. Motor Control, 1988; 2: 306-313. doi: 10.1123/mcj.2.4.306
21. Yufik YM, Friston K. Life and understanding: Origins of the understanding capacity in self-organizing nervous systems. Frontiers in the System
Neuroscience, 2016; 10: 98. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00098
22. Cavanagh PR, Kram R. Mechanical and muscular factors affecting the efficiency of human movement. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 1985; 17: 326-331.
23. Croft JL, Schroeder RT, Bertram JEA. The landscape of movement control in locomotion: cost, strategy, and solution. Frontiers in Psychology, 2019;
10: 716. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00716
24. Mattos D, Kuhl J, Scholz JP, Latash ML. Motor equivalence (ME) during reaching: Is ME observable at the muscle level? Motor Control, 2013;
17:145-175. doi: 10.1123/mcj.17.2.145
25. Mattos D, Latash ML, Park E, Kuhl J, Scholz JP. Unpredictable elbow joint perturbation during reaching results in multijoint motor equivalence.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 2011; 106: 1424-1436. doi: 10.1152/jn.00163.2011
26. Mattos D, Schöner G, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Motor equivalence during accurate multi-finger force production. Experimental Brain Research,
2015; 233: 487-502. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4128-1
27. Leone FC, Nottingham RB, Nelson LS. The folded normal distribution. Technometrics, 19661; 3: 543550.
28. Falaki A, Huang X, Lewis MM, Latash ML. Motor equivalence and structure of variance: Multi-muscle postural synergies in Parkinson’s disease.
Experimental Brain Research, 2017; 235: 2243-2258. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-4971-y
29. Cuadra C, Bartsch A, Tiemann P, Reschechtko S, Latash ML. Multi-finger synergies and the muscular apparatus of the hand. Experimental Brain
Research, 2018; 236: 1383-1393. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5231-5
30. De Freitas PB, Freitas SMSF, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Stability of steady hand force production explored across spaces and methods of
analysis. Experimental Brain Research, 2018; 236: 1545-1562. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5238-y
31. Scholz JP, Schöner G, Latash ML. Identifying the control structure of multijoint coordination during pistol shooting. Experimental Brain Research,
2000; 135: 382-404. doi: 10.1007/s002210000540
32. Latash ML, Scholz JF, Danion F, Schöner G. Structure of motor variability in marginally redundant multi-finger force production tasks. Experimental
Brain Research, 2001; 141: 153-165. doi: 10.1007/s002210100861
33. Scholz JP, Danion F, Latash ML, Schöner G. Understanding finger coordination through analysis of the structure of force variability. Biological
Cybernetics, 2002; 86: 29-39. doi: 10.1007/s004220100279
34. Zatsiorsky VM, Li ZM, Latash ML. Enslaving effects in multi-finger force production. Experimental Brain Research, 2000; 131: 187-195. doi:
10.1007/s002219900261
35. Danion F, Schöner G, Latash ML, Li S, Scholz JP, Zatsiorsky VM. A force mode hypothesis for finger interaction during multi-finger force production
tasks. Biological Cybernetics, 2003; 88: 91-98. doi: 10.1007/s00422-002-0336-z
36. Abolins V, Stremoukhov A, Walter C, Latash ML. On the origin of finger enslaving: Control with referent coordinates and effects of visual feedback.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 2020; 124: 1625-1636. doi: 10.1152/jn.00322.2020
37. Abolins V, Latash ML. The nature of finger enslaving: New results and their implications. Motor Control, 2021; 25; 680-703. doi: 10.1123/mc.2021-
0044
38. Krishnamoorthy V, Latash ML, Scholz JP, Zatsiorsky VM. Muscle synergies during shifts of the center of pressure by standing persons.
Experimental Brain Research, 2003; 152: 281-292. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1574-6
39. Danna-Dos-Santos A, Slomka K, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Muscle modes and synergies during voluntary body sway. Experimental Brain
Research, 2007; 179: 533-550. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0812-0
40. Madarshahian S, Letizi J, Latash ML. Synergic control of a single muscle: The example of flexor digitorum superficialis. Journal of Physiology, 2021;
599: 1261-1279. doi: 10.1113/JP280555
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
12 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
41. Latash ML, Madarshahian S, Ricotta J. Intra-muscle synergies: Their place in the neural control hierarchy. Motor Control, 2023; 27: 402-441. doi:
10.1123/mc.2022-0094
42. Latash ML. Motor synergies and the equilibrium-point hypothesis. Motor Control, 2010; 14: 294-322. doi: 10.1123/mcj.14.3.294
43. Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM. Joint stiffness: Myth or reality? Human Movement Science, 1993; 12: 653-692. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(93)90010-M
44. Ambike S, Mattos D, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Synergies in the space of control variables within the equilibrium-point hypothesis. Neuroscience,
2016; 315: 150-161. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.12.012
45. Nardon M, Pascucci F, Cesari P, Bertucco M, Latash ML. Synergies stabilizing vertical posture in spaces of control variables. Neuroscience, 2022;
500: 79-94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2022.08.006
46. Müller H, Sternad D. A randomization method for the calculation of covariation in multiple nonlinear relations: illustrated with the example of goal-
directed movements. Biological Cybernetics, 2023; 89: 22-33. doi: 10.1007/s00422-003-0399-5
47. Bosco G, Poppele RE. Encoding of hindlimb kinematics by spinocerebellar circuitry. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 2002; 140: 185-192.
48. Gorniak S, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Hierarchies of synergies: An example of the two-hand, multi-finger tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 2007;
179: 167-180. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0777-z
49. Arbib MA, Iberall T, Lyons D. Coordinated control programs for movements of the hand. In: Goodwin AW and Darian-Smith I, eds. Hand Function
and the Neocortex. Berlin: Springer Verlag; 198; pp. 111-129. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-70105-4_7
50. Gorniak S, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Hierarchical control of static prehension: II. Multi-digit synergies. Experimental Brain Research, 2009; 194: 1-
15. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1663-7
51. Latash ML. Optimality, stability, and agility of human movement: New optimality criterion and trade-offs. Motor Control, 2023; 27: 123-159. doi:
10.1123/mc.2021-0135
52. Olafsdottir H, Yoshida N, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Anticipatory covariation of finger forces during self-paced and reaction time force production.
Neuroscience Letters, 2005; 381: 92-96. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.003
53. Krishnan V, Aruin AS, Latash ML. Two stages and three components of postural preparation to action. Experimental Brain Research, 2011; 212: 47-
63. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2694-z
54. Freitas SM, Scholz JP, Stehman AJ. Effect of motor planning on use of motor abundance. Neuroscience Letters, 2007; 417: 66-71. doi:
10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.037
55. Tillman M, Ambike S. Cue-induced changes in the stability of finger force-production tasks revealed by the uncontrolled manifold analysis. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 2018; 119: 21-32. doi: 10.1152/jn.00519.2017
56. Tillman M, Ambike S. The influence of recent actions and anticipated actions on the stability of finger forces during a tracking task. Motor Control,
2020; 24: 365-382. doi: 10.1123/mc.2019-0124
57. Piscitelli D, Falaki A, Solnik S, Latash ML. Anticipatory postural adjustments and anticipatory synergy adjustments: Preparing to a postural
perturbation with predictable and unpredictable direction. Experimental Brain Research, 2017; 235: 713-730. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4835-x
58. De SD, Ambike S, Latash ML. Two aspects of feed-forward control of action stability: Effects of action speed and unexpected events. Experimental
Brain Research, 2024; 242(9):2177-2191. doi: 10.1007/s00221-024-06892-x.
59. Martin V, Scholz JP, Schöner G. Redundancy, self-motion, and motor control. Neural Computations, 2009; 21:1371-1414. doi:
10.1162/neco.2008.01-08-698
60. Martin V, Reimann H, Schöner G. A process account of the uncontrolled manifold structure of joint space variance in pointing movements. Biological
Cybernetics, 2019; 113: 293-307. doi: 10.1007/s00422-019-00794-w
61. Latash ML, Shim JK, Smilga AV, Zatsiorsky V. A central back-coupling hypothesis on the organization of motor synergies: a physical metaphor and
a neural model. Biological Cybernetics, 2005; 92: 186-191. doi: 10.1007/s00422-005-0548-0
62. Madarshahian S, Latash ML. Effects of hand muscle function and dominance on intra-muscle synergies. Human Movement Science, 2022; 82:
102936. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2022.102936
63. Madarshahian S, Ricotta J, Latash ML. Intra-muscle synergies stabilizing reflex-mediated force changes. Neuroscience, 2022; 505: 59-77. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroscience.2022.10.009
64. De SD, Ricotta JM, Benamati A, Latash ML. Two classes of action-stabilizing synergies reflecting spinal and supraspinal circuitry. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 2024; 131: 152-165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00352.2023
65. Sainburg RL. Handedness: differential specializations for control of trajectory and position. Exercise and Sport Science Reviews, 2005; 33: 206-213.
doi: 10.1097/00003677-200510000-00010
66. Vaillancourt DE, Russell DM. Temporal capacity of short-term visuomotor memory in continuous force production. Experimental Brain Research,
2002; 145:275-285. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1081-1
67. Ambike S, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Processes underlying unintentional finger force changes in the absence of visual feedback. Experimental
Brain Research, 2015; 233: 711-721. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4148-x
68. Latash ML, Huang X. Neural control of movement stability: Lessons from studies of neurological patients. Neuroscience, 2015; 301: 39-48. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.075
69. Reisman D, Scholz JP. Aspects of joint coordination are preserved during pointing in persons with post-stroke hemiparesis. Brain, 2003; 126: 2510-
2527. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg246
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
13 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
70. Jo HJ, Maenza C, Good DC, Huang X, Park J, Sainburg RL, Latash ML. Effects of unilateral stroke on multi-finger synergies and their feed-forward
adjustments. Neuroscience, 2016; 319: 194-205. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.054
71. Gera G, Freitas SM, Scholz JP. Relationship of diminished interjoint coordination after stroke to hand path consistency. Experimental Brain
Research, 2016; 234: 741-751. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4500-9
72. Gera G, McGlade KE, Reisman DS, Scholz JP. Trunk muscle coordination during upward and downward reaching in stroke survivors. Motor
Control, 2016; 20: 50-69. doi: 10.1123/mc.2014-0038
73. Vaz DV, Pinto VA, Junior RRS, Mattos DJS, Mitra S. Coordination in adults with neurological impairment - A systematic review of uncontrolled
manifold studies. Gait and Posture, 2019; 69: 66-78. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2019.01.003
74. Benamati A, Ricotta JM, De SD, Latash ML. Three levels of neural control contributing to performance-stabilizing synergies in multi-finger tasks.
Neuroscience, 2024; 551: 262-275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2024.05.044
75. Scholz JP, Kang N, Patterson D, Latash ML. Uncontrolled manifold analysis of single trials during multi-finger force production by persons with and
without Down syndrome. Experimental Brain Research, 2003; 153: 45-58. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1580-8
76. Singh T, SKM V, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML .Fatigue and motor redundancy: Adaptive increase in force variance in multi-finger tasks. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 2010; 103: 2990-3000. doi: 10.1152/jn.00077.2010
77. Ricotta JM, De SD, Nardon M, Benamati A, Latash ML. Effects of fatigue on intra-muscle force-stabilizing synergies. Journal of Applied Physiology,
2023; 135: 1023-1035. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00419.2023
78. Olafsdottir H, Yoshida N, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Elderly show decreased adjustments of motor synergies in preparation to action. Clinical
Biomechanics, 2007; 22: 44-51. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2006.08.005
79. Olafsdottir H, Zhang W, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Age related changes in multi-finger synergies in accurate moment of force production tasks.
Journal of Applied Physiology, 2007; 102: 1490-1501. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00966.2006
80. Lewis MM, Lee E-Y, Jo HJ, Park J, Latash ML, Huang X. Synergy as a new and sensitive marker of basal ganglia dysfunction: A study of
asymptomatic welders. Neurotoxicology, 2016; 56: 76-85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2016.06.016
81. Park J, Wu Y-H, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Changes in multi-finger interaction and coordination in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 2012; 108: 915-924. doi: 10.1152/jn.00043.2012
82. Falaki A, Huang X, Lewis MM, Latash ML. Impaired synergic control of posture in Parkinson’s patients without postural instability. Gait and Posture,
2016; 44: 209-215. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.12.035
83. Freitas SMSF, de Freitas PB, Falaki A, Corson T, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Synergic control of action in levodopa-naïve Parkinson’s disease
patients. II. Multi-muscle synergies stabilizing vertical posture. Experimental Brain Research, 2020; 238: 2931-2945. doi: 10.1007/s00221-020-
05947-z
84. De Freitas PB, Freitas SMSF, Reschechtko S, Corson T, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Synergic control of action in levodopa-naïve Parkinson’s
disease patients: I. Multi-finger interaction and coordination. Experimental Brain Research, 2020; 238: 229-245. doi: 10.1007/s00221-019-05709-6
85. Park J, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Dopaminergic modulation of motor coordination in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism and Related
Disorders, 2014; 20: 64-68. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2013.09.019
86. Falaki A, Huang X, Lewis MM, Latash ML. Dopaminergic modulation of multi-muscle synergies in postural tasks performed by patients with
Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology, 2017; 33: 20-26. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2017.01.002
87. Falaki A, Jo HJ, Lewis MM, O’Connell B, De Jesus S, McInerney J, Huang X, Latash ML. Systemic effects of deep brain stimulation on synergic
control in Parkinson’s disease. Clinical Neurophysiology, 2018; 129: 1320-1332. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2018.02.126
88. Falaki A, Cuadra C, Lewis MM, Prado-Rico JM, Huang X, Latash ML. Multi-muscle synergies in preparation to gait initiation in Parkinson’s disease.
Clinical Neurophysiology, 2023; 154: 12-24. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2023.06.022
89. Domkin D, Laczko J, Jaric S, Johansson H, Latash ML. Structure of joint variability in bimanual pointing tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 2002;
143: 11-23. doi: 10.1007/s00221-001-0944-1
90. Latash ML, Yarrow K, Rothwell JC. Changes in finger coordination and responses to single pulse TMS of motor cortex during practice of a multi-
finger force production task. Experimental Brain Research, 2003; 151: 60-71. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1480-y
91. Kang N, Shinohara M, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Learning multi-finger synergies: An uncontrolled manifold analysis. Experimental Brain Research,
2004; 157: 336-350. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1850-0
92. Wu Y-H, Pazin N, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Practicing elements vs. practicing coordination: Changes in the structure of variance. Journal of Motor
Behavior, 2012; 44: 471-478. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2012.740101
93. Wu Y-H, Pazin N, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Improving finger coordination in young and elderly persons. Experimental Brain Research, 2013; 226:
273-283. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3433-4
94. Rosenblatt NJ, Hurt CP, Latash ML, Grabiner MD. An apparent contradiction: Increasing variability to achieve greater precision? Experimental Brain
Research, 2014; 232: 403-413. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3748-1
95. Jo HJ, Lucassen E, Huang X, Latash ML. Changes in multi-digit synergies and their feed-forward adjustments in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Motor
Behavior, 2017; 49: 218-228. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2016.1169986
96. Wu Y-H, Latash ML. The effects of practice on coordination. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 2014; 42: 37-42. doi:
10.1249/JES.0000000000000002
BJMB
Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior
Latash
2024
VOL.18
https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
14 of 14
Special issue:
Using the Framework of the uncontrolled manifold
(UCM) to understand motor behavior
97. Latash ML, Shim JK, Zatsiorsky VM. Is there a timing synergy during multi-finger production of quick force pulses? Experimental Brain Research,
2004; 159: 65-71. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1933-y
98. Goodman SR, Shim JK, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Motor variability within a multi-effector system: Experimental and analytical studies of multi-
finger production of quick force pulses. Experimental Brain Research, 2005; 163: 75-85. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2147-z
99. Klishko AN, Farrell BJ, Beloozerova IN, Latash ML, Prilutsky BI. Stabilization of cat paw trajectory during locomotion. Journal of Neurophysiology,
2014; 112: 1376-1391. doi: 10.1152/jn.00663.2013
100. Mangalam M, Rein R, Fragaszy DM. Bearded capuchin monkeys use joint synergies to stabilize the hammer trajectory while cracking nuts in
bipedal stance. Proceedings of the Royal Society Biological Sciences, 2018; 285 (1889): 20181797. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.1797
101. Reschechtko S, Latash ML. Stability of hand force production: I. Hand level control variables and multi-finger synergies. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 2017; 118: 3152-3164. doi: 10.1152/jn.00485.2017
102. Zhang W, Scholz JP, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. What do synergies do? Effects of secondary constraints on multi-digit synergies in accurate
force-production tasks. Journal of Neurophysiology, 2008; 99: 500-513. doi: 10.1152/jn.01029.2007
103. Zhang W, Sainburg RL, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Hand dominance and multi-finger synergies. Neuroscience Letters, 2006; 409: 200-204.
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.09.048
104. De Freitas PB, Freitas SMSF, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Individual preferences in motor coordination seen across the two hands:
Relations to movement stability and optimality. Experimental Brain Research, 2019; 237: 1-13. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5393-1
105. Abolins V, Latash ML. Unintentional force drifts across the human fingers: Implications for the neural control of finger tasks. Experimental
Brain Research, 2022; 240: 751-761. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06287-2
106. Piscitelli D, Buttram A, Abernathy K, Canelón J, Knighten D, Solnik S. Clinically relevant estimation of minimal number of trials for the
uncontrolled manifold analysis. Journal of Biomechanics, 2024; 171: 112195. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2024.112195
107. Piscitelli D, Buttram A, Gibson S, Hager J, Thomas B, Solnik S. Test-Retest reliability and measurement error of the uncontrolled manifold
analysis: A step towards the clinical translation. Journal of Biomechanics, 2024; 162: 111902. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2023.111902
Editor-in-chief: Dr Fabio Augusto Barbieri - São Paulo State University (UNESP), Bauru, SP, Brazil.
Associate editors: Dr José Angelo Barela - São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, SP, Brazil; Dr Natalia Madalena Rinaldi - Federal University of Espírito Santo
(UFES), Vitória, ES, Brazil; Dr Renato de Moraes University of São Paulo (USP), Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil.
Copyright:© 2024 Latash and BJMB. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-Non Commercial-No Derivatives 4.0
International License which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: Nothing to declare.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.20338/bjmb.v18i1.433
Citation: Latash ML. (2024). Brief History of the Uncontrolled Manifold Hypothesis and Its Role in Motor Control. Brazilian Journal of Motor Behavior, 18(1):e433.